References

 

Ach, N. (1905).  Über die Willenstätigkeit und das Denken.  Göttingen: Vandenhoeck and Ruprecht.

Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J.F.X., and Goodale, M.A. (1995).  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand.  Current Biology, 5, 679-685.

Aguilón, F. de (1613).  Opticorvm libri sex.  Antuerpiae: Officina Plantiniana.

Ahsen, A. (1977).  Eidetics: An overview.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 1, 5-38.

Ahsen, A. (1985).  Unvividness paradox.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 9, 1-18.

Ahsen, A. (1986).  Prologue to unvividness paradox.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 10, 1-8.

Ahsen, A. (1987).  Epilogue to unvividness paradox.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 11, 13-60.

Ahsen, A. (1990).  AA-VVIQ and imagery paradigm: Vividness and unvividness issue in VVIQ research paradigms.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 14, 1-58.

Albutt, J., Ling, J., Heffernan, T.M., and Shafiullah, M. (2008).  Self report imagery questionnaire scores and subtypes of social-desirable responding: Auditory imagery, visual imagery, and thinking style.  Journal of Individual Differences, 29, 181-188.

Allen, F. (1924).  On the reflex origin of the self light of the retina.  Journal of the Optical Society of America, 8, 275-286.

Allport, G.W. (1924).  Eidetic imagery.  British Journal of Psychology, 15, 99-120.

Allport, G.W. (1928).  The eidetic image and the after-image.  American Journal of Psychology, 40, 418-425.

Alston, W. (1971).  Varieties of privileged access.  American Philosophical Quarterly, 8, 223-241.

Amedi, A., Malach, R., and Pascual-Leone, A., (2005).  Negative BOLD differentiates visual imagery and perception.  Neuron, 48, 859-872.

Ammons, C.H., Worchel, P., and Dallenbach, K.M. (1953).  ‘Facial vision’: The perception of obstacles out of doors by blindfolded and blindfolded-deafened subjects.  American Journal of Psychology, 66, 519-554.

Andrews, G.A., and Calkins, M.W. (1900).  Minor studies from the psychological laboratory of Wellesley College: Studies of the dream consciousness.  American Journal of Psychology, 12, 131-134.

Angell, J.R. (1910).  Methods for the determination of mental imagery.  Psychological Monographs, 13, 61-108.

Angell, J.R. (1911).  Imageless thought.  Psychological Review, 18, 295-323.

Antonietti, A. (1999).  Can students predict when imagery will allow them to discover the problem solution?  European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 407-428.

Antonietti, A., Bologna, D. and Lupi, G. (1997).  Creative synthesis of visual images is not associated with individual differences.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 881-882.

Antony, M.V. (2008).  Are our concepts conscious state and conscious creature vague.  Erkenntnis, 68, 239-263.

Aristotle (4th c. BCE/1906).  De sensu and de memoria, trans. G.R.T. Ross.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Aristotle (4th c. BCE/1936).  On the soul, parva naturalia, on breath, trans. W.S. Hett.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Aristotle (4th c. BCE/1996).  Aristotle on sleep and dreams, ed. D. Gallop.  Warminster, England: Aris and Phillips.

Armstrong, D.M. (1955).  Illusions of sense.  Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 33, 88-106.

Armstrong, D.M. (1963).  Is introspective knowledge incorrigible?  Philosophical Review, 72, 417-432.

Armstrong, D.M. (1981).  The nature of mind.  Ithaca, NY: Cornell.

Ashmead, D.H., and Wall, R.S. (1999). Auditory perception of walls via spectral variations in the ambient sound field.  Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 36, 313-322.

Aubert, H. (1865).  Physiologie der Netzhaut.  Breslau: Morgenstern.

Austin, J.L. (1962).  Sense and sensibilia.  London: Oxford.

Avant, L.L. (1965).  Vision in the Ganzfeld.  Psychological Bulletin, 64, 246-258.

Ayer, A.J. (1940).  The foundations of empirical knowledge.  London: MacMillan.

Baars, B.J. (1988).  A cognitive theory of consciousness.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Baars, B.J. (1997).  In the theater of consciousness.  New York: Oxford.

Bar-On, D. (2004).  Speaking My Mind.  Oxford: Oxford.

Barry, H., Jr., and Bousfield, W.A. (1934).  Implications of the flight of colors.  Psychological Review, 41, 300-305.

Bates, W.H. (1944).  The Bates method for good sight without glasses.  London: Faber and Faber.

Bayle, P. (1702/1734-8).  The dictionary historical and critical of Mr. Peter Bayle, trans. des Maizeaux.  London: Knapton.

Beaunis, H. (1903).  Contribution à la Psychologie du Rêve.  American Journal of Psychology, 14 (3/4), 7-23.

Bellamy, R. (1915).  The analysis of a nightmare.  Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 10, 11-18.

Belot, G. (2003).  Remarks on the geometry of visibles.  Philosophical Quarterly, 53, 581–6.

Bem, D.J. (1972).  Self-perception theory.  Advances in experimental and social psychology, 6, 1-62.

Bentley, M. (1915). The study of dreams.  American journal of psychology, 26, 196-210.

Berger, R.J. (1963).  Experimental modification of dream content by meaningful verbal stimuli.  British Journal of Psychiatry, 109, 722-740.

Berkeley, G. (1710/1965), A treatise concerning the principles of human knowledge.  In Principles, dialogues, and philosophical correspondence, ed. C.M. Turbayne.  New York: Macmillan.

Berry, W. (1922).  The flight of colors in the after image of a bright light.  Psychological Bulletin, 19, 307-337.

Berry, W. (1927).  Color sequences in the after-image of a white light.  American Journal of Psychology, 38, 584-598.

Betts, G.H. (1909).  The distribution and functions of mental imagery.  New York: Teachers College, Columbia.

Bexton, W.H., Heron, W., and Scott, T.H. (1954).  Effects of decreased variation in the sensory environment.  Canadian Journal of Psychology, 8, 70-76.

Blackmore, S. (2002).  There is no stream of consciousness.  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9 (no. 5-6), 17-28.

Blajenkova, O., Kozhevnikov, M., and Motes, M.A. (2006).  Object-spatial imagery: A new self-report imagery questionnaire.  Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 239-263.

Block, N. (1995).  On a confusion about the function of consciousness.  Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 227-287.

Block, N. (2007).  Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience.  Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 481-548.

Block, N. (ed., 1981).  Imagery.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Blum, H. P. (1964).  Colour in dreams.  International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 45, 519-529.

Bolton, R. (1978). Salience of color terms in the dreams of Peruvian mestizos and Qolla Indians.  Journal of Social Psychology, 105, 299-300.\

Bonnet, C. (1760/1769).  Essai analytique sur les faculties de l’ame, 2nd ed., vol. 2.  Copenhague: Philibert.

Borges, J.L. (1949/1962).  The Zahir.  In Labyrinths.  New York: New Directions.

Boring, E.G. (1921).  The stimulus error.  American Journal of Psychology, 32, 449-471.

Boring, E.G. (1927).  Edward Bradford Titchener.  American Journal of Psychology, 38, 489-506.

Boring, E.G. (1929/1950).  A history of experimental psychology, 2nd ed.  East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Boring, E.G. (1942).  Sensation and Perception in the History of Experimental Psychology.  New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Boring, E.G. (1953).  A history of introspection.  Psychological Bulletin, 50, 169-189.

Bornstein, R.F. (1989).  Subliminal techniques as propaganda tools: Review and critique.  Journal of Mind and Behavior, 10, 231-262.

Bower, G.H. (1972).  Mental imagery and associative learning.  In Cognition in Learning and Memory, ed. L.W. Gregg.  New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Brandstätter, H. (2001).  Time sampling diary: An ecological approach to the study of emotion in everyday life situations.  In Persons, Situations, and Emotions, ed. H. Brandstätter and A. Eliasz.  Oxford: Oxford.

Braun, A.R., Balkin, T.J., Wesensten, N.J., Gwadry, F., Carson, R.E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Belenky G., and Herscovitch, P. (1998).  Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye movement sleep.  Science, 279, 91-95.

Brentano, F.C. (1874/1973).  Psychology from an empirical standpoint, ed. O. Kraus and L.L. McAlister.  London: Routledge.

Brewer, W.F., and Schommer-Aikins, M. (2006).  Scientists are not deficient in mental imagery: Galton revisited.  Review of General Psychology, 10, 130-146.

Broad, C.D. (1925).  The mind and its place in nature.  New York: Humanities Press.

Brown, J.L. (1965).  Afterimages.  In Vision and visual perception, ed. C.H. Graham.  New York: Wiley.

Bruner, J.S., and Postman, L. (1949).  On perception of incongruity: A paradigm.  Journal of Personality, 18, 206-223.

Bühler, K. (1907).  Tatsachen und Probleme zu einer Psychologie der Denvorgänge.  Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie, 9, 297-365.

Burbridge, D. (1994).  Galton’s 100: An exploration of Francis Galton’s imagery studies.  British Journal for the History of Science, 27, 443-463.

Burge, T. (1988).  Individualism and self-knowledge.  Journal of Philosophy, 85, 649-63.

Burge, T. (1996).  Our entitlement to self-knowledge.  Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 96, 91-116.

Burgess, C., and Lund, K. (2000). The dynamics of meaning in memory. In Cognitive dynamics: Conceptual and representational change in humans and machines, ed. E. Dietrich and A.B. Markman. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Burton, L.J., and Fogarty, G.J. (2003).  The factor structure of visual imagery and spatial abilities.  Intelligence, 31, 289-318.

Calef, V. (1954).  Color in dreams.  Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 2, 453-461.

Calkins, M.W. (1893).  Minor studies from the psychological laboratory of Clark University: Statistics of dreams.  American Journal of Psychology, 5, 311-343.

Campos, A. and Fernández (1997).  Imagen mental e intervalo de retención en el recuerdo de series.  Estudios de Psicología, 58, 105-112.

Campos, A. and Pérez, M.J. (1997).  Mnemonic images and associated pair recall.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 21, 73-82.

Campos, A., and Fernández (1998).  La imagen mental en el sistema de enlace.  Revista de Psicología, 16, 3-17.

Campos, A., Pérez, M.J. and González, M.A. (1997).  The interactiveness of paired images is affected by image bizarreness and image vividness.  Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 16, 301-307.

Carretti, B., Borella, E., and De Beni, R. (2007).  Does strategic memory training improve the working memory performance of younger and older adults?  Experimental Psychology, 54, 311-320.

Carroll, J. B., Davies, P., and Richman, B. (1967).  The American Heritage word frequency book. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Carruthers, P. (2009).  How we know our own minds: The relationship between mindreading and metacognition.  Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 121-182.

Chalmers, D.J. (1996).  The conscious mind.  New York: Oxford.

Chalmers, D.J. (2003).  The content and epistemology of phenomenal belief.  In Consciousness: New philosophical essays, ed. Q. Smith and A. Jokic.  Oxford: Clarendon.

Chara, P.J. (1992).  Some concluding thoughts on the debate about the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 947-954.

Cherry, E.C. (1953).  Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and two ears.  Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975-979.

Chisholm, R.M. (1957).  Perceiving.  Ithaca, NY: Cornell.

Churchland, P.M. (1985).  Reduction, qualia, and the direct introspection of brain states.  Journal of Philosophy, 82, 8-28.

Churchland, P.M. (1988), Matter and Consciousness, Rev. Ed.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Clark, A. (2001).  Visual experience and motor action: Are the bonds too tight?  Philosophical Review, 110, 495-519.

Cleomedes (circa 2nd c. CE/2004).  Cleomedes’ lectures on astronomy, ed. A.C. Bowen and R.B. Todd.  Berkeley, CA: University of California.

Cohen, P. (2008).  The World Science Festival: Oliver Sacks at the MET.  New York Times: Arts Beat (May 30, http://artsbeat.blogs.nytimes.com).

Coltheart, M., and Glick, M.J. (1974).  Visual imagery: A case study.  Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 438-453.

Comte, A. (1830).  Cours de philosophie positive, vol. 1.  Paris: Bachelier.

Coren, S., and Girgus, J.S. (1978).  Seeing is deceiving.  Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Cornoldi, C. (1995).  Imagery and meta-imagery in the VVIQ.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 19, 131-136.

Cotzin, M., and Dallenbach, K.M. (1950).  “Facial Vision:” The Role of Pitch and Loudness in the Perception of Obstacles by the Blind.  American Journal of Psychology, 63, 485-515.

Crawford, H.J. and Allen, S.N. (1996).  Paired-associate learning and recall of high and low imagery words: Moderating effects of hypnosis, hypnotic susceptibility level and visualization abilities.  American Journal of Psychology, 109, 353-372.

Crick. F. (1994).  The astonishing hypothesis.  New York: Scribner.

Cui, X., Jeter, C.B., Yang, D., Montague, P.R., and Eagleman, D.M. (2007).  Vividness of mental imagery: Individual variability can be measured objectively.  Vision Research, 47, 474-478.

da Vinci, L. (1519/1989).  Leonardo on Painting, ed. M. Kemp.  New Haven: Yale.

Dahl, H. (1979).  Word frequencies in spoken American English.  Detroit: Gale Research.

Danquah, A.N., Farrell, M.J., and O’Boyle, D.J. (2008).  Biases in the subjective timing of perceptual events: Libet et al. (1983) revisited.  Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 616-627.

de Martino, M.F. (1953)  Sex differences in the dreams of Southern college students.  Journal of Clinical Psychology, 9, 199-201.

Dean, G.M., and Morris, P.E. (2003).  The relationship between self-reports of imagery and spatial ability.  British Journal of Psychology, 94, 245-273.

Delboeuf, J. (1865).  Note sur certaines illusions d’optique: Essai d’une théorie psychphysique de la manière dont l’oeil apprécie les distances et les angles.  Bulletins de l’Académie Royale des Sciences, Lettres et Beaux-arts de Belgique, 19 (2d serie), 195-216.

Dennett, D.C. (1969).  Content and consciousness.  New York: Humanities.

Dennett, D.C. (1991).  Consciousness explained.  Boston: Little, Brown, and Company.

Dennett, D.C. (2001).  Surprise, Surprise.  Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 982.

Dennett, D.C. (2002).  How could I be wrong?  How wrong could I be?  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9 (no. 5-6), 13-6.

Dennett, D.C. (2007).  Heterophenomenology reconsidered.  Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6, 247-270.

Descartes, R. (1641/1984). Meditations on first philosophy. In The philosophical writings of Descartes, vol. II, trans. J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, and D. Murdoch.  Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Descartes, R. (1649/1985). The passions of the soul.  In The philosophical writings of Descartes, vol. I, trans. J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, and D. Murdoch.  Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dewey, John (1886).  Psychology.  New York: Harper.

Dharmakīrti (7th c./1958).  Nyāya-bindu.  In T. Stcherbatsky, Buddhist Logic.  The Hague: Mouton.

Diderot, D.  (1749/1916).  Letter on the blind for the use of those who see.  In Diderot’s early philosophical works, ed. M. Jourdain.  Chicago: Open Court.

Dignāga (6th c./1968).  Pramānasamuccaya.  In M. Hattori, Dināga, On Perception.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Doust, J.W.L. (1951).  Studies in the physiology of awareness: The incidence and content of dream patterns and their relationship to anoxia.  Journal of Mental Science, 97, 801-811.

Dretske, F. (1995).  Naturalizing the mind.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Dretske, F. (2000).  Perception, knowledge, and belief.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Dretske, F. (2003).  How do you know you are not a zombie?  In Privileged access, ed. B. Gertler.  Aldershot, England: Ashgate.

Dreyfus, G.B.J. (1997).  Recognizing reality.  Albany, NY: SUNY.

Eberman, C., and McKelvie, S.J. (2002).  Vividness of visual imagery and source memory for audio and text.  Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 87-95.

Einstein, G.O. and McDaniel, M.A. (1987).  Distinctiveness and the mnemonic benefits of bizarre imagery.  In Imagery and related mnemonic processes, ed. M.A. McDaniel and M. Pressley.  New York: Springer-Verlag.

Ellis, H. (1911).  The world of dreams.  New York: Houghton Mifflin.

Epicurus (3rd c. BCE/1926).  The extant remains, trans. C. Bailey.  Oxford: Oxford.

Epstein, W., Park, J., and Casey, A. (1961).  The current status of the size-distance hypotheses.  Psychological Bulletin, 58, 491-514.

Ermentrout, G.B., and Cowan, J.D. (1979).  A mathematical theory of visual hallucination patterns.  Biological Cybernetics, 34, 137-150.

Ernest, C.H. (1977).  Imagery ability and cognition: A critical review.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 2, 181-216.

Eton, D.T., Gilner, F.H. and Munz, D.C. (1998).  The measurement of imagery vividness: A test of the reliability and validity of the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire and the Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 22, 125-136.

Euclid (c. 300 BCE/1945).  The optics of Euclid.  Journal of the Optical Society of America, 35, 357-372.

Evans, R.B. (1972).  Titchener and his lost system.  Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 8, 168-180.

Evans, W.E. (1973).  Echolocation by marine delphenids and one species of fresh-water dolphin.  Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54, 191-199.

Faw, B. (1997).  Outlining a brain model of mental imaging abilities.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 21, 283-288.

Faw, B. (2009).  Conflicting intuitions may be based on differing abilities.  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16 (no. 4), 45-68.

Fechner, G.T. (1860/1889).  Elemente der Psychophysik, vol 2.  Leipzig: Breitkopf and Härtel.

Fechner, G.T. (1860/1966).  Elements of Psychophysics, vol. 1, ed. D.H. Howes and E.G. Boring.  New York: Holt.

Feldman, M., Todman, L., and Bender, M.B. (1974).  “Flight of colours” in lesions of the visual system.  Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 37, 1265-1272.

Ferree, C.E. (1908).  The intermittence of minimal visual sensations: I.  American Journal of Psychology, 19, 58-129.

Fick, A. (1879).  Die Lehre von der Lichtempfindungen.  In Handbuch der Physiologie, vol. 3, part 1, ed. L. Hermann.  Leipzig: F.C.W. Vogel.

Flanagan, O. (1992).  Consciousness reconsidered.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Fortier, R.H. (1951).  A study of the relationship of response to color and some personality functions.  Ph.D. dissertation, Case Western Reserve.

Frayn, D.H. (1991). The incidence and significance of perceptual qualities in the reported dreams of patients with anorexia nervosa.  Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 517-520.

Freud, S. (1900/1931).  The interpretation of dreams, trans. A.A. Brill.  New York: Carlton House.

Fröhlich, F.W. (1921).  Untersuchungen über periodische Nachbilder.  Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 52, 60-88.

Furst, C.J. (1979).  The inside and outside of eidetic imagery.  Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 602-603.

Galton, F. (1880).  Statistics of mental imagery.  Mind, 5, 301-318.

Galton, F. (1883/1907).  Inquiries into human faculty and its development, 2nd ed.  London: J.M. Dent.

Galton, F. (1908).  Memories of my life.  London: Methuen.

Ganis, G., Thompson, W.L., and Kosslyn, S.M. (2004).  Brain areas underlying visual imagery and visual perception: an fMRI study.  Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 226-241.

Gardner, R.W., and Long, R.I. (1960a).  Errors of the standard and illusion effects with L-shaped figures.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 10, 107-109.

Gardner, R.W., and Long, R.I. (1960b).  Errors of the standard and illusion effects with the inverted-T.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 10, 47-54.

Garma, A. (1961). Colour in dreams.  International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 42, 556-559.

Gegenfurtner, K.R., and Kiper, D.C. (2003).  Color vision.  Annual Review of Neuroscience, 26, 181-206.

Geldard, F.A., and Sherrick, C.E. (1972).  The cutaneous “rabbit”: A perceptual illusion.  Science, 178 (no. 4057), 178-179.

Gemignani, A., Sebastiani, L., Simoni, A., Santarcangelo, E.L., and Ghelarducci, B. (2006).  Hyponotic trait and specific phobia: EEG and autonomic output during phobic stimulation.  Brain Research Bulletin, 69, 197-203.

Gertler, B. (2001).  Introspecting phenomenal states.  Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 63, 305-28.

Gertler, B. (ed. 2003).  Privileged access.  Aldershot, England: Ashgate.

Gibson, J.J., and Waddell, D. (1952).  Homogeneous retinal stimulation and visual perception.  American Journal of Psychology, 65, 263-270.

Gilinsky, A.S. (1951).  Perceived size and distance in visual space.  Psychological Review, 58, 460-482.

Girgus, J.J., Rock, I., and Egatz, R. (1977).  The effect of knowledge of reversibility on the reversibility of ambiguous figures.  Perception and Psychophysics, 22, 550-556.

Glicksohn, J., Steinbach, I. and Elimalach-Malmilyan, S. (1999).  Cognitive dedifferentiation in eidetics and synaesthesia: Hunting for the ghost once more.  Perception, 28, 109-120.

Glover, S. (2002).  Visual illusions affect planning but not control.  Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 288-292.

Goethe, J.W. von (1810/1840/1967).  Goethe’s Theory of Colours, trans. C.L. Eastlake.  London: Frank Cass.

Goldman, A. (1986).  Epistemology and cognition.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Goldman, A. (2004).  Epistemology and the evidential status of introspective reports.  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11 (no. 7-8), 1-16.

Goldman, A. (2006).  Simulating minds.  New York: Oxford.

González, M., Campos, A. and Pérez, M.J. (1997).  Mental imagery and creative thinking.  Journal of Psychology, 131, 357-364.

Goodale, M.A., Gonzales, C.L.R., Króliczak, G. (2008).  Action rules: Why the visual control of reaching and grasping is not always influenced by perceptual illusions.  Perception, 37, 355-366.

Gopnik, A., and Graf, P. (1988).  Knowing how you know: Young children’s ability to identify and remember the sources of their beliefs.  Child Development, 59, 1366-1371.

Gordon, M.S., and Rosenblum, L.D. (2004).  Perception of sound-obstructing surfaces using body-scaled judgments.  Ecological Psychology, 16, 87-113.

Gordon, R. (1949).  An investigation into some of the factors that favour the formation of stereotyped images.  British Journal of Psychology, 40, 156-167.

Grandin, T. (1995).  Thinking in pictures.  New York: Doubleday.

Granrud, C.E. (2009).  Development of size constancy in children: A test of the metacognitive theory.  Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 71, 644-654.

Gray, C.R. and Gummerman, K. (1975).  The enigmatic eidetic image: A critical examination of methods, data, and theories.  Psychological Bulletin, 82, 383-407.

Gregory, R.L. (1970).  The intelligent eye.  New York: McGraw-Hill.

Griffin, D. (1958).  Listening in the dark.  New Haven: Yale.

Guillot, A., Champely, S., Batier, C., Thiriet, P., and Collet, C. (2007).  Relationship between spatial abilities, mental rotation, and functional anatomy learning.  Advances in Health Sciences Education, 12, 491-507.

Gur, M. (1989).  Color and brightness fade-out in the Ganzfeld is wavelength dependent.  Vision Research, 29, 1335-1341.

Gyselinck, V., Meneghetti, C., De Beni, R., and Pazzaglia, F. (2009).  The role of working memory in spatial text processing: What benefit of imagery strategy and visuospatial abilities?  Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 12-20.

Haber, R.N. (1979).  Twenty years of haunting eidetic imagery: Where’s the ghost?  Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 583-629.

Haber, R.N. and Haber, L. (2000).  Experiencing, remembering, and reporting events.  Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 6, 1057-1097.

Haber, R.N. and Haber, R.B. (1964).  Eidetic imagery: I. Frequency.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 19, 131-138.

Haffenden, A.M., and Goodale, M.A. (1998).  The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception.  Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 122-136.

Hall, C. S. (1951). What people dream about.  Scientific American, 184 (no. 5), 60-63.

Hall, C. S., and Van de Castle, R. L. (1966). The content analysis of dreams.  New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Hall, D.E. (1980/2002).  Musical Acoustics, 3rd Ed.  Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Thomson Learning.

Hall, D.E. (1981).  The difference between difference tones and rapid beats.  American Journal of Physics, 49, 632-636.

Hanson, N.R. (1969).  Perception and discovery.  San Francisco: Freeman, Cooper.

Harman, G. (1990).  The intrinsic quality of experience.  Philosophical Perspectives, 4, 31-52.

Hatfield, G. (2009).  Perception and cognition.  New York: Oxford.

Hausfeld, S., Power, R.P., Gorta, A., and Harris, P. (1982).  Echo Perception of Shape and Texture by Sighted Subjects.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 623-632.

Haybron, D. (2008).  The pursuit of unhappiness.  Oxford: Oxford.

Hayes, S.P. (1935).  Facial vision or the sense of obstacles.  Watertown, MA: Perkins.

Heaps, C. and Nash, M. (1999).  Individual differences in imagination inflation.  Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 313-318.

Heavey, C.L., and Hurlburt, R.T. (2008).  The phenomena of inner experience.  Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 798-810.

Helmholtz, H. (1856/1909/1962).  Helmholtz’s treatise on physiological optics, ed. J.P.C. Southall.  New York: Dover.

Hering, E. (1868/1977).  The theory of binocular vision, ed. B. Bridgeman and L. Stark.  New York: Plenum.

Hering, E. (1878).  Zur Lehre vom Lichtsinne.  Wien: Carl Gerold’s Sohn.

Hering, E. (1905).  Grundzüge der Lehre von Lichtsinn, vol. 1.  Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann.

Hering, E. (1920/1964).  Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense, trans. L.M. Hurvich and D. Jameson.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Herman, J., Roffwarg, H., and Tauber, E. S. (1968).  Color and other perceptual qualities of REM and NREM sleep.  Psychophysiology, 5, 223.

Heron, W. (1961).  Cognitive and physiological effects of perceptual isolation.  In Sensory deprivation, ed. P. Solomon, P.E. Kubzansky, P.H. Leiderman, J. Mendelson, R. Trumbull and D. Wexler.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Higashiyama, A., and Shimono, K. (1994).  How accurate is size and distance perception for very far terrestrial objects?  Function and causality.  Perception and Psychophysics, 55, 429-442.

Hills, P.J., Elward, R.L., and Lewis. M.B. (2008).  Identity adaptation is mediated and moderated by visualisation ability.  Perception, 37, 1241-1257.

Hine, R. (forthcoming).  Attention as experience: Through “thick” and “thin”.  Journal of Consciousness Studies.

Hintikka, J. (1962).  Cogito ergo sum: Inference or performance?  Philosophical Review, 71, 3-32.

Hochberg, J.E. (1950).  Figure-ground reversal as a function of visual satiation.  Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 682-686.

Hochberg, J.E., Triebel, W., and Seaman, G. (1951).  Color adaptation under conditions of homogeneous visual stimulation (Ganzfeld).  Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 153-159.

Hochel, M., and Milán, E.G. (2008).  Synaesthesia: The existing state of affairs.  Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 93-117.

Hochman, J. (2002).  Memory and the eidetic.  New York: Brandon House.

Holender, D. (1986).  Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal.  Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1-66.

Holender, D., and Duscherer, K. (2004).  Unconscious perception: The need for a paradigm shift.  Perception and Psychophysics, 66, 872-881.

Homuth, P. (1913).  Beiträge zur Kenntnis der Nachbilderscheinungen.  Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie, 26, 181-268.

Horgan, T. and Tienson, J. (2002).  The intentionality of phenomenology and the phenomenology of intentionality.  In Philosophy of Mind, ed. D. J. Chalmers.  New York: Oxford.

Horgan, T., and Kriegel, U. (2007).  Phenomenal epistemology: What is consciousness that we may know it so well?  Philosophical Issues, 17, 123-144.

Horgan, T., Tienson, J., and Graham, G. (2005).  Internal-world skepticism and the self-presentational nature of phenomenal consciousness.  In Experience and analysis: Proceedings of the 27th International Wittgenstein Symposium, ed. M. Reicher and J. Marek, 191-207.  Wien: ÖBV and HPT.

Horowitz, M.J. (1970).  Image formation and cognition.  London: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Huet, P.-D. (1694/2003).  Against Cartesian philosophy, ed. T.M. Lennon.  New York: Humanity Books.

Hume, D. (1740/1978).  A treatise of human nature, ed. L.A. Selby-Bigge and P.H. Nidditch.  Oxford: Oxford.

Hume, D. (1748/1975).  An enquiry concerning human understanding.  In Enquiries concerning human understanding and concerning the principles of morals, ed. L.A. Selby-Bigge and P.H. Nidditch.  Oxford: Clarendon.

Humphrey, G. (1951).  Thinking.  London: Methuen.

Hurlburt, R.T. (1990).  Sampling normal and schizophrenic inner experience.  New York: Plenum.

Hurlburt, R.T., and Akhter, S.A. (2008).  Unsymbolized thinking.  Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1364-1374.

Hurlburt, R.T., and Heavey, C.L. (2004).  To beep or not to beep: Obtaining accurate reports about awareness.  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11 (no. 7-8), 113-128.

Hurlburt, R.T., and Heavey, C.L. (2006).  Exploring inner experience.  Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Hurlburt, R.T., and Schwitzgebel, E. (2007).  Describing inner experience? Proponent meets skeptic.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Hurvich, L.M., and Jameson, D. (1966).  The perception of brightness and darkness.  Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Husband, R.W. (1935).  Sex differences in dream contents.  Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 30, 513-521.

Huxley, A. (1956).  Heaven and Hell.  New York: Harper.

Huxley, T.H. (1895).  Hume.  London: Macmillan.

Ichikawa, J. (2009).  Dreaming and imagination.  Mind and Language, 24, 103-121.

Intons-Peterson, M.J. (1983).  Imagery paradigms: How vulnerable are they to experimenters’ expectations?  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 394-412.

Isaac, A.R. and Marks, D.F. (1994).  Individual differences in mental imagery experience: Developmental changes and specialization.  British Journal of Psychology, 85, 479-500.

Jack, A.I, and Shallice, T. (2001).  Introspective physicalism as an approach to the science of consciousness.  Cognition 79, 161-196.

Jackson, F. (1977).  Perception.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Jaensch, E.R. (1923).  Über den Aufbau der Wahrnehmungswelt.  Leipzig: Johann Ambrosium Barth.

Jaensch, E.R. (1923).  Über den Aufbau der Wahrnehmungswelt.  Leipzig: Barth.

Jaensch, E.R. (1930).  Eidetic Imagery, trans. O. Oeser.  London: Kegan Paul.

James, W. (1884).  On some omissions of introspective psychology.  Mind, O.S., 9, 1-26.

James, W. (1890/1981).  The principles of psychology.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Jankowski, W.L., Dee, S.C., and Cartwright, R.D. (1977).  A distribution of colorimetric imagery in REM sleep.  Sleep Research, 6, 123.

Jaynes, J. (1976).  The origin of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind.  Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Johansson, S., and Hofland, K. (1989). Frequency analysis of English vocabulary and grammar. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Judd, D.B. (1927).  A quantitative investigation of the Purkinje after-image.  American Journal of Psychology, 38, 507-533.

Kahn, E., Dement, W., Fisher, C., and Barmack, J. E. (1962).  Incidence of color in immediately recalled dreams.  Science, 137, 1054-1055.

Kaylor, C.W. and Davidson, R.S. (1979).  Accuracy of recall as a function of eidetic imagery.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 48, 1143-1148.

Kellogg, W.N. (1958).  Echo ranging in the porpoise.  Science, 128, 982-988.

Kells, K. (2001).  Ability of blind people to detect obstacles in unfamiliar environments.  Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 33, 153-157.

Kelly, S. (2008).  Content and constancy: Phenomenology, psychology, and the content of perception.  Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 76, 682-690.

Kenet, T., Bibitchkov, D., Tsodyks, M., Grinvald, A., and Arieli, A. (2003).  Spontaneously emerging cortical representations of visual attributes.  Nature, 425 (no. 6961), 954-956.

Keng, C.-J., and Lin, H.-Y. (2006).  Impact of telepresence levels on internet advertising effects.  CyberPsychology and Behavior, 9, 82-94.

Keogh, L. and Markham, R. (1998).  Judgements of other people’s memory reports: Differences in reports as a function of imagery vividness.  Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 159-171.

Kessler, R.C., DuPont, R.L., Berglund, P., Wittchen, H.-U. (1999).  Impairment in pure and comorbid generalized anxiety disorder and major depression at 12 months in two national surveys.  American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1915-1923.

Kilgour, A.R., and Lederman, S.J. (2002).  Face recognition by hand.  Perception and Psychophysics, 54, 339-352.

Kind, A. (2003).  What’s so transparent about transparency?  Philosophical Studies, 115, 225-244.

Kish, D. (2009).  Human echolocation: How to “see” like a bat.  New Scientist, 201 (no. 2703, April 11), 31-33.

Klüver, H. (1933).  Eidetic imagery.  In A Handbook of Child Psychology, 2nd Ed. 2, ed. C. Murchison.  New York: Russell and Russell.

Knapp, P.H. (1953).  The ear, listening, and hearing.  Journal of the American Psychiatric Association, 1, 672-689.

Knapp, P.H. (1956).  Sensory impressions in dreams.  Psychoanalytic quarterly, 25, 325-347.

Koch, C. (2004).  The quest for consciousness.  Englewood, CO: Roberts.

Koch, C., and Tsuchiya, N. (2007).  Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes.  Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 16-22.

Kogon, M.M., Jasiukaitis, P., Berardi, A., Gupta, M., Kosslyn, S.M. and Spiegel, D. (1998).  Imagery and hypnotizability revisited.  International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 46, 363-370.

Köhler, P. (1912).  Beiträge zur systematischen Traumbeobachtung.  Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie, 23, 415-483.

Köhler, W. (1929/1947).  Gestalt psychology.  New York: Liveright.

Köhler, W., and Wallach, H. (1944).  Figural aftereffects; an investigation of visual processes.  Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 88, 269-357.

Koninski, K. (1934).  Beitrag zur Kenntnis der entoptischen Erscheinungen.  Kwartalnik Psychologiczny, 5, 337-382.

Kornblith, H. (1998).  What is it like to be me?  Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 76, 48-60.

Kosslyn, S.M. (1980).  Image and mind.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Kosslyn, S.M. (1994). Image and brain.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Kosslyn, S.M., Ganis, G., and Thompson, W.L. (2001).  Neural foundations of imagery.  Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635-642.

Kosslyn, S.M., Shin, L.M., Thompson, W.L., McNally, R.J., Rauch, S.L., Pitman, R.K., and Alpert, N.M. (1996).  Neural effects of visualizing and perceiving aversive stimuli: A PET investigation.  NeuroReport, 7, 1569-1576.

Kozhevnikov, M., Kosslyn, S., and Shephard, J. (2005).  Spatial versus object visualizers: A new characterization of visual cognitive style.  Memory and Cognition, 33, 710-726.

Kubie, L.S. (1950).  Practical and theoretical aspects of psychoanalysis.  Oxford: International Universities.

Kučera, H., and Francis, W. N. (1967).  Computational analysis of present-day American English.  Providence: Brown.

Külpe, O. (1893/1895).  Outlines of Psychology, trans. E.B. Titchener.  London: Swan Sonnenschein.

Kundt, A. (1863).  Untersuchungen über Augenmaass und optische Täuschungen.  Annalen der Physik und Chemie, 120, 118-158.

Künnapas, T.M. (1955).  An analysis of the “horizontal-vertical illusion”.  Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 134-140.

Kunzendorf, R.G. (1984).  Centrifugal effects of eidetic imaging on flash electroretinograms and autonomic responses.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 8, 67-76.

Kunzendorf, R.G., Young, K., Beecy, T. and Beals, K. (2000).  Is visual thinking “imageless thought”?  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91, 981-982.

Kusch, M. (1999).  Psychological knowledge.  London: Routledge.

Ladd, G.T. (1892).  Contribution to the psychology of visual dreams.  Mind, N.S., 1, 299-304.

Ladd, G.T. (1894).  Direct control of the retinal field.  Psychological Review, 1, 351-355.

Ladd, G.T. (1903).  Direct control of the “retinal field”: Report on three cases.  Psychological Review, 10, 139-149.

Laeng, B., and Teodorescu, D.-S. (2002).  Eye scanpaths during visual imagery reenact those of perception of the same visual scene.  Cognitive Science, 26, 207-231.

Lamme, V.A.F. (2003).  Why visual attention and awareness are different.  Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 12-18.

Lamme, V.A.F. (2005).  The difference between visual attention and awareness: A cognitive neuroscience perspective.  In The neurobiology of attention, ed. L. Itti, G. Rees, and J.K. Tsotsos.  Amsterdam: Elsevier. 

Lamme, V.A.F., and Roelfsema, P.R. (2000).  The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing.  Trends in Neuroscience, 23, 571-579.

Lanca, M., and Bryant, D.J. (2001).  Metric representations of haptically explored triangles.  American Journal of Psychology, 114, 377-409.

Lavie, N. (2006).  The role of perceptual load in visual awareness.  Brain Research, 1080, 91-100.

Le Conte, J. (1881/1897).  Sight: An exposition of the principles of monocular and binocular vision, 2nd ed.  New York: Appleton.

Leask, J., Haber, R.N. and Haber, R.B. (1969).  Eidetic imagery in children: II Longitudinal and experimental results.  Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 3, 25-48.

Lee, D.N. (1990).  Getting around with light or sound.  In Perception and control of self-motion, ed. R. Warren and A.H. Wertheim.  Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Lee, H.S., and Dobbins, A.C. (2006).  Perceiving surfaces in depth beyond the fusion limit of their elements.  Perception, 35, 31-39.

Lehar, S. (2003).  Gestalt isomorphism and the primacy of subjective conscious experience: A Gestalt Bubble model.  Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 375-444.

Lequerica, L., Rapport, L., Axelrod, B.N., Telmet, K., and Whitman, R.D. (2002).  Subjective and objective assessment methods of mental imagery control: Construct validation of self-report measures.  Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 1103-1116.

Lewis, C.I. (1946).  An analysis of knowledge and valuation.  La Salle, IL: Open Court.

Lewis, M.B., and Ellis, H.D. (2000).  Satiation in name and face recognition.  Memory and Cognition, 28, 783-788.

Libet, B. (1985).  Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action.  Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 529-566.

Liebowitz, H.W., Pollard, S.W., and Dickson, D. (1967).  Monocular and binocular size-matching as a function of distance at various age-levels.  American Journal of Psychology, 80, 263-268.

Lobmaier, J.S., and Mast, F.W. (2008).  Face imagery is based on featural representations.  Experimental Psychology, 55, 47-53.

Locke, J. (1690/1975).  An essay concerning human understanding, ed. P.H. Nidditch.  Oxford: Clarendon.

Loftus, E.F. (1979).  Eyewitness testimony.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Long, G.M., and Olszweski, A.D. (1999).  To reverse or not to reverse: When is an ambiguous figure not ambiguous?  American Journal of Psychology, 112, 41-71.

López, A. T., Sanchéz, T. J., Arriaga, A. E., and Saldivar, R. E. (1986).  El rol de la memoria visual y el color onirico en la frecuencia del recuerdo de sueños.  Revista mexicana de psicología, 3, 143-149.

Lucretius (1st c. BCE/1910).  Lucretius on the nature of things, trans. C. Bailey.  London: Oxford.

Luria, A.R. (1965/1968).  The mind of a mnemonist.  New York: Basic Books.

Lutz, A., Dunne, J.D., and Davidson, R.J. (2007).  Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness.  In Cambridge handbook of consciousness, ed. P. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, and E. Thompson.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Lycan, W.G. (1996).  Consciousness and experience.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Mach, E. (1886/1959).  The analysis of sensations, trans. C.M. Williams.  New York: Dover.

MacIntyre, T., Moran, A., and Jennings, D.J. (2002).  Is controllability of imagery related to canoe-slalom performance?  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 1245-1250.

Mack, A., and Rock, I. (1998).  Inattentional blindness.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Malach, R. (2006).  Perception without a perceiver, in conversation with Zoran Josipovic.  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 13 (no. 9), 57-66.

Malcolm, N. (1959).  Dreaming.  London: Routledge and Paul.

Malebranche, N. (1674/1997).  The search after truth, trans. T.M. Lennon.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Manser, A.R. (1956).  Dreams, part II.  Aristotelian Society: Supplementary Vol., 30, 208-228.

Marcel, T. (1980).  Conscious and preconscious recognition of polysemous words: Locating the selective effects of prior verbal context.  In Attention and Performance, 8, ed. R.S. Nickerson.

Marks, D.F. (1973).  Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures.  British Journal of Psychology, 64, 17-24.

Marks, D.F. and McKellar, P. (1982).  The nature and function of eidetic imagery.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 6, 1-124.

Massaro, D.W., and Loftus, G.R. (1996).  Sensory and perceptual storage: Data and theory.  In Memory: Handbook of Perception and Cognition, 2nd ed., ed. E.L. Bjork and R.A. Bjork.  San Diego, CA: Academic.

Mast, F.W., Merfeld, D.M., and Kosslyn, S.M. (2006).  Visual mental imagery during caloric vestibular stimulation.  Neuropsychologica, 44, 101-109.

Matilal, B.K. (1986).  Perception.  Oxford: Clarendon.

Matsuoka, K. (1989).  Imagery vividness, verbalizer-visualizer, and fantasy-proneness in young adult eidetikers.  Tohoku Psychologica Folia, 48, 25-32.

Maury, L.-F. A. (1861/1878).  Le Sommeil et les Rêves, 4th ed.  Paris: Dider.

McCarty, B., and Worchel, P. (1954).  Rate of motion and object perception in the blind.  New Outlook for the Blind, 48, 316-322;

McCrone, J. (1993).  The myth of irrationality.  New York: Carroll & Graf.

McGinn, C. (2004). Mindsight.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

McKelvie, S.J. (1995).  The VVIQ as a psychometric test of individual differences in visual imagery vividness: A critical quantitative review and plea for direction.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 19, 1-106.

Meadows, P.J. (forthcoming).  Contemporary arguments for a geometry of visual experience.  European Journal of Philosophy.  DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-0378.2009.00387.x.

Meehl, P.E. (1990).  Why summaries of research on psychological theories are often uninterpretable.  Psychological Reports, 66, 195-244.

Mencius (4th c. BCE/1970).  Mencius, trans. D.C. Lau.  London: Penguin.

Menon, G.J. (2005).  Complex visual hallucinations in the visually impaired.  Archives of Ophthalmology, 123, 349-355.

Merikle, P.M., Smilek, D., and Eastwood, J.D. (2001).  Perception without awareness: Perspectives from cognitive psychology.  Cognition, 79, 115-134.

Metzger, W. (1930).  Optische Untersuchung am Ganzfeld: II. Mitteilung: Zur Phänomenologie des homogenen Ganzfelds.  Psychologische Forschung, 13, 6-29.

Middleton, W.C. (1933).  Nocturnal dreams.  Scientific Monthly, 37, 460-464.

Middleton, W.C. (1942).  The frequency with which a group of unselected college students experience colored dreaming and colored hearing.  The journal of general psychology, 27, 221-229.

Miller, S. (Steve) and Peacock, R. (1982).  Evidence for the uniqueness of eidetic imagery.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 1219-1233.

Miller, S.C. (Stuart) (1964).  The manifest dream and the appearance of colour in dreams.  International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 45, 512-518.

Mole, C. (2008).  Attention and consciousness.  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15 (no. 4), 86-104.

Monroe, W.S. (1898).  Einige Experimente über Gesichtsbilder in Traum.  American journal of psychology, 9, 413-414.

Monson, C.K. and Hurlburt, R.T. (1993).  A comment to suspend the introspection controversy: Introspecting subjects did agree about “imageless thought.  In R.T. Hurlburt, Sampling Inner Experience in Disturbed Affect.  New York: Plenum.

Montaigne, M. (1580/1948).  The complete essays of Montaigne, trans. D.M. Frame.  Stanford, CA: Stanford.

Moore, G.E. (1953).  Some main problems of philosophy.  London: George Allen and Unwin.

Moran, R. (2001).  Authority and estrangement.  Princeton, NJ: Princeton.

Moray, N. (1959).  Attention and dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions.  Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 56-60.

Most, S.B., Scholl, B.J., Clifford, E.R., and Simons, D.J. (2005).  What you see is what you set: Sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of awareness.  Psychological Review, 112, 217-242.

Müller, G.E. (1896).  Zur Psychophysik der Gesichtsempfindungen: Kapitel 1.  Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 10, 1-82.

Müller, G.E. (1897).  Zur Psychophysik der Gesichtsempfindungen: Kapitel 4.  Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 14, 1-76.

Müller, G.E. (1904).  Die Gesichtspunkte und die Tatsachen der Psychophysischen Methodik.  Wiesbaden: J.F. Bergmann.

Müller, J. (1826/1927).  Über die Phantastischen Gesichtserscheinungen.  Leipiz: Barth.

Müller, J. (1837-1840).  Handbuch der Physiologie des Menschen für Vorlesungen.  Coblenz: Hölscher

Münsterberg, H. (1927).  On the witness stand.  New York: Clark Boardman.

Murzyn, E. (2008).  Do we only dream in colour?  A comparison of reported dream colour in older and younger adults with different experiences of black and white media.  Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1128-1237.

Nagel, T. (1974).  What is it like to be a bat?   Philosophical Review, 83, 435-450.

Nahmias, E.A. (2002).  Verbal reports on the contents of consciousness: Reconsidering introspectionist methodology.  Psyche, 8 (no. 21).

Necker, L.A. (1832).  Observations on some remarkable optical phænomena seen in Switzerland; and on an optical phænonmenon which occurs in viewing a figure of a crystal or geometrical solid.  (The London and Edinburgh) Philosophical Magazine (and Journal of Science), 3rd series, 1, 329-337.

Nicholas, S. (1995).  Joseph Delboeuf on visual illusions: A historical sketch.  American Journal of Psychology, 108, 563-574.

Nichols, S. and Grantham, R. (2001).  Adaptive complexity and phenomenal consciousness.  Philosophy of Science, 67, 648-670.

Nichols, S. and Stich, S.P. (2003).  Mindreading.  Oxford: Clarendon.

Nickerson, R.S., and Adams, M.J. (1979).  Long-term memory for a common object.  Cognitive Psychology, 11, 287-307.

Nijhawan, R. (1994).  Motion extrapolation in catching.  Nature, 370, 256-257.

Nir, Y., Hasson, U., Levy, I., Yeshurun, Y., and Malach, R. (2006).  Widespread functional connectivity and fMRI fluctuations in human visual cortex in the absence of visual stimulation.  NeuroImage, 30, 1313-1324.

Nisbett, R., and Ross, L. (1980).  Human Inference.  Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Nisbett, R.E. and Wilson, T.D. (1977).  Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes.  Psychological Review, 84, 231-259.

Noë, A. (2004).  Action in perception.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

O’Brien, G., and Opie, J. (1999).  A connectionist theory of phenomenal experience.  Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 127-196.

O’Regan, J.K. (1992).  Solving the “Real” Mysteries of Visual Perception.  The World As an Outside Memory.  Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46, 461-88.

O’Shaughnessy, B. (2003).  Consciousness and the world, new ed.  Oxford: Oxford.

Ogden, R.M. (1911).  Imageless thought: Resume and critique.  Psychological Bulletin, 8, 183-197.

Okada, H., Matsuoka, K., and Hatakeyama, T. (2005).  Individual differences in the range of sensory modalities experienced in dreams.  Dreaming, 15, 106-115.

Ono, H., Fay, A., and Tarbell, S.E. (1986).  A “visual” explanation of facial vision.  Psychological Research, 48, 57-62.

Oppel, J.J. (1854-1855).  Über geometrisch-optische Tauschungen.  Jahresbericht des Frankfurter Vereins, 37-47.

Padgham, C.A. (1975).  Colours experienced in dreams.  British Journal of Psychology, 66, 25-28.

Paine, P.A. (1980).  Eidetic imagery and recall accuracy in preschool children.  Journal of Psychology, 105, 253-258.

Paivio, A. (1971).  Imagery and verbal processes.  New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

Paivio, A. (1986).  Mental representations: A Dual Coding Approach.  New York: Oxford.

Palmer, S.E. (1999).  Vision science.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Panum, P. (1858).  Physiologische Untersuchungen über das Sehen mit zwei Augen.  Kiel: Schwerssche Buchhandlung.

Papineau, D. (1993).  Philosophical naturalism.  Oxford: Blackwell.

Peddie, W. (1922).  Colour vision.  London: Edward Arnold.

Pérez-Mata, M.N., Read, J.D., and Diges, M. (2002).  Effects of divided attention and word concreteness on correct recall and false memory reports.  Memory, 10, 161-177.

Perky, C.W. (1910).  An experimental study of imagination.  American Journal of Psychology, 21, 422-452.

Perky, C.W. (1910).  An experimental study of imagination.  American Journal of Psychology, 21, 422-452.

Peterson, M.A., and Hochberg, J. (1983).  Opposed-set measurement procedure: A quantitative analysis of the role of local cues and intention in form perception.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 183-193.

Petrova, P.K., and Cialdini, R.B. (2005).  Fluency of consumption imagery and the backfire effects of imagery appeals.  Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 442-452.

Philodemus (1st c. BCE/1978).  On methods of inference, rev. ed, ed. P.H. de Lacy and E.A. de Lacy.  Napoli: Bibliopois.

Pillsbury, W.B. (1908).  Attention.  New York: Macmillan.

Pitt, D. (2004).  The phenomenology of cognition, or what is it like to think that P?  Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 69, 1-36.

Plato (4th c. BCE/1961).  The collected dialogues, ed. E. Hamilton and H. Cairns.  Princeton, NJ: Princeton.

Plomp, R. (1976).  Aspects of tone sensation.  London: Academic Press.

Plotinus (3rd c. CE/1966-1988).  Plotinus, trans. A.H. Armstrong.  London: Heinemann.

Plutarch (c. 100 CE/1918).  Plutarch’s lives, vol. VI, trans. B. Perrin.  London: Heinemann.

Polanyi, M. (1966).  The tacit dimension.  Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

Post, R.B., O’Malley, M.D., Yeh, T.L., and Bethel, J. (2006).  On the origin of vertical line bisection errors.  Spatial Vision, 19, 505-527.

Price, H.H. (1932).  Perception.  London: Methuen.

Price, H.H. (1941).  The foundations of empirical knowledge, by Alfred J. Ayer.  Mind, 50, 280-293.

Prinz, J.J. (2004).  Gut reactions.  Oxford: Oxford.

Prinz. J. (2007).  All consciousness is perceptual.  In Contemporary debates in philosophy of mind, ed. B.P. McLaughlin and J. Cohen.  Malden: Blackwell.

Prinzmetal, W., and Gettleman, L. (1993).  Vertical-horizontal illusion: One eye is better than two.  Perception and Psychophysics, 53, 81-88.

Prior, K. (1999).  Don’t shoot the dog!  New York: Bantam.

Pritchard, R.M. (1958).  Visual illusions viewed as stabilized retinal images.  Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 77-81.

Ptolemy (2nd c. CE/1996).  Ptolemy’s theory of visual perception, trans. A.M. Smith.  Philadelphia: American Philosophical Society.

Purdy, D.M. (1939).  Vision.  In Introduction to Psychology, ed. E.G. Boring, H.S. Langfeld, and H.P. Weld.  New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Purkinje, J. (1819/2001).  In N.J. Wade and J. Brožek, Purkinje’s vision.  Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 

Purkinje, J. (1823/1919).  Beobachtungen und Versuche zur Physiologie der Sinne.  Praze: Komisi Knihkupectví J. Calve.  Reproduced in Joannes Ev-Purkinje Opera Omnia, vol. 1.  Praze.

Pylyshyn, Z.W. (1973).  What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: A critique of mental imagery.  Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1-24.

Pylyshyn, Z.W. (2002).  Mental imagery: In search of a theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 157-182.

Rao, S. (1998).  Perceptual error.  Honolulu: University of Hawai’i.

Rapport, N. (1949).  Overture to dreams.  Psychiatric quarterly, 23, 266-276.

Rechtschaffen, A., and Buchignani, C. (1992).  The visual appearance of dreams.  In The Neuropsychology of Sleep and Dreaming, ed. J.S. Antrobus and M. Bertini.  Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Reid, T. (1764/1997).  An inquiry into the human mind, ed. D.R. Brookes.  University Park, PA: Pennsylvania State.

Reisberg, D., Pearson, D.G., and Kosslyn, S.M. (2003).  Intuitions and introspections about imagery: The role of imagery experience in shaping an investigator’s theoretical views.  Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 147-160.

Rensink, R.A. (2000).  When good observers go bad: Change blindness, inattentional blindness, and visual experience.  Psyche, 6 (no. 9).

Rensink, R.A. (2004).  Visual sensing without seeing.  Psychological Science, 15, 27-32.

Rensink, R.A., O’Regan, J.K., and Clark, J.J. (1997).  To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes.  Psychological Science, 8, 368-373.

Rensink, R.A., O’Regan, J.K., and Clark, J.J. (2000).  On the failure to detect changes in scenes across brief interruptions.  Visual Cognition, 7, 127-145.

Rice, C.E. (1967).  Human echo perception.  Science, 155, 656-664;

Richardson, A. and Di Francesco, J. (1985).  Stability, accuracy, and eye movements in eidetic imagery.  Australian Journal of Psychology, 37, 51-64.

Richardson, J.T.E. (1980).  Mental imagery and human memory.  New York: St. Martin’s.

Richet, C. (1887).  L’Homme et l’Intelligence.  Paris: Germer Baillière.

Riske, M.E., Wallace, B. and Allen, P.A. (2000).  Imagining ability and eyewitness accuracy.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 24, 137-148.

Robertson, V.M., and Fry, G.A. (1937).  After-images observed in complete darkness.  American Journal of Psychology, 49, 265-276.

Robinson, J.O. (1972).  The Psychology of Visual Illusion.  London: Hutchinson.

Robinson, W.S. (2005).  Thoughts without distinctive non-imagistic phenomenology.  Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 70, 534-60.

Rock, I., Linnett, C.M., Grant, P., and Mack, A. (1992).  Perception without attention: Results of a new method.  Cognitive Psychology, 24, 502-534.

Rosenblum, L.D., and Robart, R.L. (2007).  Hearing silent shapes: Identifying the shape of a sound-obstructing surface.  Ecological Psychology, 19, 351-366.

Rosenblum, L.D., Gordon, M.S., and Jarquin, L. (2000).  Echolocating distance by moving and stationary listeners.  Ecological Psychology, 12, 181-206.

Rosenthal, R., and Fode, K.L. (1963).  The effect of experimenter bias on the performance of the albino rat.  Behavioral Science, 8, 183-189.

Rosnow, R.L., and Rosenthal, R. (1997).  People studying people.  New York: W.H. Freeman.

Rossing, T.D., Moore, F.R., and Wheeler, P.A. (1982/2002).  The science of sound, 3rd ed.  San Francisco: Addison Wesley.

Rozemond, M. (2004).  Critical notice: Janet Broughton, Descartes’s method of doubt.  Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 34, 591-614.

Russell, B. (1912).  The problems of philosophy.  London: Oxford.

Ryle, G. (1949).  The concept of mind.  New York: Barnes and Noble.

Sacks, O. (1995).  An anthropologist on Mars.  New York: Random House.

Sacks, O. (2006).  Stereo Sue.  New Yorker (June 19), 64-73.

Sanches, F. (1581/1988).  That nothing is known, ed. and trans. E. Limbrick and D.F.S. Thomson.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Sanford, E.C. (1892).  A laboratory course in physiological psychology (third paper).  American Journal of Psychology, 4, 474-490.

Sanford, E.C. (1901).  A course in experimental psychology.  Boston: Heath.

Sanford, E.C. (1917/1982).  Professor Sanford’s morning prayer.  In Memory observed, ed. U. Neisser.  San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.

Santarcangelo, E.L., Cavallaro, E., Mazzoleni, S., Marano, E., Ghelarducci, B., Dario, P., Micera, S., and Sebastiani, L. (2005).  Kinematic strategies for lowering of upper limbs during suggestions of heaviness: A real-simulator design.  Experimental Brain Research, 162, 35-45.

Sartre, J.-P. (1940/1972).  The psychology of imagination, ed. M. Warnock.  London: Methuen.

Schacter, D.C. (1976).  The hypnagogic state: A critical review of the literature.  Psychological Bulletin, 83, 452-481.

Schienle, A., Schäfer, A, and Vaitl, D. (2008).  NeuroReport, 19, 527-530.

Schön, W. (1876).  Zur Lehre vom binocularen indirecten Sehen.  Albrecht von Graefes Archiv für Ophthalmologie, 22 (no. 4), 31-62.

Schön, W. (1878a).  Zur Lehre vom binocularen Sehen.  II. Aufsatz.  Albrecht von Graefes Archiv für Ophthalmologie, 24 (no. 1), 27-130.

Schön, W. (1878b).  Zur Lehre vom binocularen Sehen.  III. Aufsatz.  Albrecht von Graefes Archiv für Ophthalmologie, 24 (no. 4), 47-116.

Schooler, J. and Schreiber, C.A. (2004).  Experience, meta-consciousness, and the paradox of introspection.  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11 (no 7-8), 17-39.

Schredl, M., Fuchedzhieva, A., Hëmig, H., and Schindele, V. (2008).  Do we think dreams are black and white due to memory problems?  Dreaming, 18, 175-180.

Schreiber, K.M., Hillis, J.M., Filippini, H.R., Schor, C.M., and Banks, M.S. (2008).  The surface of the empirical horopter.  Journal of Vision, 8 (3) 7, 1-20.

Schwitzgebel, E. (2002a).  How well do we know our own conscious experience?  The case of visual imagery.  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9 (no. 5-6), 35-53.

Schwitzgebel, E. (2002b).  Why did we think we dreamed in black and white?  Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 33, 649-660.

Schwitzgebel, E. (2003).  Do people still report dreaming in black and white?  An attempt to replicate a questionnaire from 1942.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 96, 25-29.

Schwitzgebel, E. (2004).  Introspective training apprehensively defended: Reflections on Titchener’s lab manual.  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11 (no. 7-8), 58-76.

Schwitzgebel, E. (2005).  Difference tone training: A demonstration adapted from Titchener’s Experimental Psychology.  Psyche, 11 (no. 6).

Schwitzgebel, E. (2006).  Do things look flat?  Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 72, 589-599.

Schwitzgebel, E. (2007a).  Do you have constant tactile experience of your feet in your shoes? Or is experience limited to what’s in attention?  Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14 (no. 3), 5-35.

Schwitzgebel, E. (2007b).  No unchallengeable epistemic authority, of any sort, regarding our own conscious experience – contra Dennett?  Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6, 107-13.

Schwitzgebel, E. (2008).  The unreliability of naive introspection.  Philosophical Review, 117, 245-273.

Schwitzgebel, E. (2009).  Introspection.  Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.

Schwitzgebel, E. (forthcoming-a).  Introspection, what?

Schwitzgebel, E. (forthcoming-b).  Self-unconsciousness.

Schwitzgebel, E., and Gordon, M.S. (2000).  How well do we know our own conscious experience?  The case of human echolocation.  Philosophical Topics, 28, 235-246.

Schwitzgebel, E., Huang, C., and Zhou, Y. (2006).  Do we dream in color? Cultural variations and skepticism.  Dreaming, 16, 36-42.

Scripture, E.W. (1897).  Cerebral light.  Science, N.S., 6 (no. 134), 138-139.

Searle, J.R. (1984).  Minds, Brains, and Science.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Searle, J.R. (1992).  The rediscovery of the mind.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Searle, J.R. (1993).  The problem of consciousness.  Consciousness and Cognition, 2, 310-319.

Sebastiani, L., Simoni, A., Gemignani, A., Ghelarducci, B., and Santarcangelo, E.L. (2003).  Autonomic and EEG correlates of emotional imagery in subjects with different hypnotic susceptibility.  Brain Research Bulletin, 60, 151-160. 

Sextus Empiricus (2nd c. CE/1933).  Trans. R.G. Bury.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Sextus Empiricus (2nd c. CE/1994).  Outlines of skepticism, trans. J. Annas and J. Barnes.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Sextus Empiricus (2nd c. CE/1996).  The skeptic way, trans. B. Mates.  New York: Oxford.

Sextus Empiricus (2nd c. CE/2005).  Against the logicians, trans. R. Bett.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Shaw, G.A., and DeMers, S.T. (1986).  The relationship of imagery to originality, flexibility and fluency in creative thinking.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 10, 65-74.

Sheehan, P.W. (1967).  A shortened form of Betts’ questionnaire upon mental imagery.  Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, 386-389.

Shoemaker, S. (1963).  Self-knowledge and self-identity.  Ithaca: Cornell.

Shoemaker, S. (1996).  The first-person perspective and other essays.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Shuey, A.M. (1924).  The flight of colors.  American Journal of Psychology, 35, 559-582.

Siewert, C. (1998).  The significance of consciousness.  Princeton: Princeton.

Siewert, C. (2004).  Is experience transparent?  Philosophical Studies, 117, 15-41.

Siewert, C. (2006).  Is the appearance of shape Protean?  Psyche, 12 (no. 3).

Simmons, J.A. (1989).  A view of the world through a bat’s ear.  Cognition, 33, 155-199.

Simner, J., Mulvenna, C., Sagiv, N., Tsakanikos, E., Witherby, S.A., Fraser, C., Scott, K., and Ward, J. (2006).  Synaesthesia: The prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences.  Perception, 35, 1024-1033.

Simons, D.J. (2000).  Attentional capture and inattentional blindness.  Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 147-155.

Simons, D.J., and Chabris, C.F. (1999).  Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events.  Perception, 28, 1059-1074.

Simons, D.J., and Levin, D.T. (1998).  Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction.  Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 644-649.

Skinner, B.F. (1945).  The operational analysis of psychological terms.  Psychological Review, 52, 270-277.

Skinner, B.F. (1953).  Science and human behavior.  New York: Macmillan.

Smeets, J.B.J., and Brenner, E. (2006).  10 years of illusions.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1501-1504.

Smythies, J.R. (1957).  A preliminary analysis of the stroboscopic patterns.  Nature, 179 (no. 4558), 523-524.

Snodgrass, M., Bernat, E., and Shevrin, H. (2004).  Unconscious perception: A model-based approach to method and evidence.  Perception and Psychophysics, 66, 846-867.

Snyder, F. (1970).  The phenomenology of dreaming.  In The psychodynamic implications of the physiological studies on dreams, ed. L. Madow and L. H. Snow.  Springfield, IL: Thomas.

Solomon, S.G, and Lennie, P. (2007).  The machinery of colour vision.  Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 276-286.

Sommer, R. (1978).  The mind’s eye.  New York: Delacourte.

Sommer, R. (1980).  Strategies for imagery research.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 4, 115-121.

Sorenson, R. (2004).  We see in the dark.  Noûs, 38, 456-480.

Spence, C., Shore, D.I., and Klein, R.M. (2001).  Multisensory prior entry.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 799-832.

Spener, M. (forthcoming).  Disagreement about cognitive phenomenology.  In Cognitive phenomenology, ed. T. Bayne and M. Montague.  Oxford: Oxford.

Sperling, G. (1960).  The information available in brief visual presentation.  Psychological Monographs, 74 (no. 11).

Srinivasan, N. (2008).  Interdependence of attention and consciousness.  Progress in Brain Research, 168, 65-75.

Stamper, D.A., Lund, D.J., Mochany, J.W., and Stuck, B.E. (2000).  Laser-induced afterimages in humans.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91, 15-33.

Stepansky, R., Holzinger, B., Schmeiser-Rieder, A., Saletu, B., Kunze, M., and Zeitlhofer, J. (1998).  Austrian dream behavior: Results of a representative population survey. Dreaming, 8, 23-30.

Stickney, S.E., and Englert, T.J. (1975).  The ghost flute.  The Physics Teacher, 13, 518-522.

Stoffregen, T.A., and Pittenger, J.B. (1995).  Human echolocation as a basic form of perception and action.  Ecological Psychology, 7, 181-216.

Stout, G.F. (1899/1977).  A manual of psychology, ed. D.N. Robinson.  Washington, DC: University Publications of America.

Stwertka, S. (1993).  The stroboscopic patterns as dissipative structures.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 17, 69-78.

Suedfeld, P. (1980).  Restricted environmental stimulation.  New York: Wiley.

Suinn, R.M. (1966).  Jungian personality typology and color dreaming.  New York (State) Mental Hygiene Department Psychiatric Quarterly, 40, 659-666.

Supa, M., Cotzin, M., and Dallenbach, K.M. (1944). “Facial Vision”: The Perception of Obstacles by the Blind.  American Journal of Psychology, 62, 133-183.

Tapia, F., Werboff, J., and Winokur, G. (1958).  Recall of some phenomena of sleep.  Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 127, 119-123.

Tatibana, Y. (1938).  Grundtypen der Traumfarben.  Tohoku Psychologica Folia, 6, 127-144.

Tauber, E.S., and Green, M.R. (1962).  Color in dreams.  American Journal of Psychotherapy, 16, 221-229.

Taya, R., and Ohinata, S. (2002).  Afterimage oscillation after a brief light flash.  Japanese Psychological Research, 44, 99-106.

Thomas, I.E. (1956).  Dreams, Part I.  Aristotelian Society: Supplementary Vol., 30, 197-207.

Thomas, N. (1989).  Experience and theory as determinants of attitudes toward mental representation: The case of Knight Dunlap and the vanishing images of J. B. Watson.  American Journal of Psychology, 102, 395-412.

Thomas, N. (1999). Are theories of imagery theories of imagination? An active perception approach to conscious mental content.  Cognitive science, 23, 207-245.

Titchener, E.B. (1896/1906).  An outline of psychology, rev. ed.  New York: Macmillan.

Titchener, E.B. (1898/1900).  A primer of psychology, rev. ed. New York: Macmillan.

Titchener, E.B. (1901-1905).  Experimental psychology: A manual of laboratory practice.  New York: Macmillan.

Titchener, E.B. (1901).  Idio-retinal light.  In Dictionary of philosophy and psychology, ed. J. M. Baldwin.  New York: Macmillan.

Titchener, E.B. (1908).  Lectures on the elementary psychology of feeling and attention.  New York: Macmillan.

Titchener, E.B. (1909).  Lectures on the experimental psychology of the thought-processes.  New York: Macmillan.

Titchener, E.B. (1910).  A text-book of psychology.  New York: Macmillan.

Titchener, E.B. (1912a).  Prolegomena to a study of introspection.  American Journal of Psychology, 23, 427-448.

Titchener, E.B. (1912b).  The schema of introspection.  American Journal of Psychology, 23, 485-508.

Titchener, E.B. (1915).  A beginner’s psychology.  New York: Macmillan.

Titchener, E.B. (1929).  Systematic psychology: Prolegomena.  Ithaca, NY: Cornell.

Tomes, J.L. and Katz, A.N. (1997).  Habitual susceptibility to misinformation and individual differences in eyewitness memory.  Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 233-251.

Tomes, J.L. and Katz, A.N. (2000).  Confidence-accuracy relations for real and suggested events. Memory, 8, 273-283.

Trappey, C. (1996).  A meta-analysis of consumer choice and subliminal advertising.  Psychology and Marketing, 13, 517-530.

Troland, L.T. (1922/2008).  The present status of visual science.  BiblioBazaar.

Tye, M. (1991). The imagery debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Tye, M. (1995).  Ten problems of consciousness.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Tye, M. (2000).  Color, consciousness, and content.  Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Tye, M. (2003).  Representationalism and the transparency of experience.  In Privileged access, ed. B. Gertler.  Aldershot, England: Ashgate.

Unger, P. (1975).  Ignorance.  London: Clarendon.

Van Cleve, J. (2002).  Thomas Reid’s geometry of visible.  Philosophical Review, 111, 373–416.

Vernon, J., Marton, T., and Peterson, E. (1961).  Sensory deprivation and hallucinations.  Science, 133 (no. 3467), 1808-1812.

Volkmann, A.W. (1846).  Sehen.  In Handwörterbuch der Physiologie, vol. 3, part 1, ed. R. Wagner.  Braunschweig: Friedrich Bieweg und Sohn.

Wade, A., Augath, M., Logothetis, N., Wandell, B. (2008).  fMRI measurement of color in macaque and human.  Journal of Vision, 8 (10) 6, 1-19.

Walczyk, J.J. (1995).  Between- versus within-subjects assessments of image vividness.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 19, 161-176.

Walczyk, J.J. and Taylor, R.W. (2000).  Reverse-spelling, the VVIQ, and mental imagery.  Journal of Mental Imagery, 24, 177-188.

Wallace, B. (1979).  Hypnotic susceptibility and the perception of afterimages and dot stimuli.  American Journal of Psychology, 92, 681-691.

Wallace, B. (1990).  Imagery vividness, hypnotic susceptibility, and the perception of fragmented stimuli.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58,  354-359.

Wallace, B., Allen, P.A. and Propper, R.E. (1996).  Hypnotic susceptibility, imaging ability, and anagram-solving activity.  International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 44, 324-337.

Wang, K., Jiang, T., Yu., C., Tian, L., Li, J., Liu, Y., Zhou, Y., Xu, L., Song, M., and Li, K. (2008).  Spontaneous activity associated with primary visual cortex: A resting-state fMRI study.  Cerebral Cortex, 18, 697-704.

Wang, Yangming (early 16th c./1963).  Instructions for practical living and other neo-Confucian writings, trans. W.-t. Chan.  New York: Columbia.

Ward, J. (1918).  Psychological principles.  Cambridge: Cambridge.

Washburn, M.F. (1899).  Subjective colors and the afterimage: Their significance for the theory of attention.  Mind, N.S., 8, 25-34.

Wasinger, K., Zelhart, P.F. and Markley, R.P. (1982).  Memory for random shapes and eidetic ability.  Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 1076-1078.

Watson, J.B. (1913).  Psychology as the behaviorist views it.  Psychological Review, 20, 158-77.

Wehrle, R., Czisch, M., Kaufmann, C., Wetter, T.C., Holsboer, F., Auer, D.P., Pollmächer, T. (2005).  Rapid eye movement-related brain activation in human sleep: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Neuroreport, 16, 853-857.

Weve, H. (1925).  The colours of after-images.  British Journal of Ophthalmology, 9, 627-638.

Wheatstone, C. (1838/1983).  Contributions to the physiology of vision – Part the first.  On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision.  In Brewer and Wheatstone on vision, ed. N.J. Wade.  London: Academic.

Wilson, R.A. (2003).  Intentionality and phenomenology.  Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 84, 413-31.

Wilson, T.D. (2002).  Strangers to ourselves.  Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

Winograd, E., Peluso, J.P. and Glover, T.A. (1998).  Individual differences in susceptibility to memory illusions.  Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, S5-S27.

Wittgenstein, L. (1967).  Zettel, ed. G.E.M. Anscombe and G.H. von Wright.  Berkeley, CA: University of California.

Wolgast, E.H. (1958).  Perceiving and impressions.  Philosophical Review, 67, 226-236.

Worchel, P., and Dallenbach, K.M. (1947).  “Facial Vision:” Perception of Obstacles by the Deaf-Blind.  American Journal of Psychology, 60, 502-553

Wright, W. (2005).  Distracted drivers and unattended experience.  Synthese, 144, 41-68.

Wundt, W. (1888).  Selbstbeobachtung und innere Wahrnehmung.  Philosophische Studien, 4, 292-309.

Wundt, W. (1896/1897).  Outlines of psychology, trans. C.H. Judd.  New York: Stechert.

Wundt, W. (1907). Uber Ausfrageexperimente und uber die Methoden zur Psychologie des Denkens.  Psychologische Studien, 3, 301-360.

Wundt, W. (1908a).  Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie, vol. 1.  Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann.

Wundt, W. (1908b).  Kritische Nachlese zur Ausfragemethode.  Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie, 9, 445-459.

Wyra, M., Lawson, M.J., and Hungi, N. (2007).  The mnemonic keyword method: The effects of bidirectional keyword training and of ability to image on foreign language vocabulary recall.  Learning and Instruction, 17, 360-371.

Xitco, M.J., and Roitblat, H.L. (1996).  Object recognition through eavesdropping: Passive echolocation in bottlenose dolphins.  Animal Learning and Behavior, 24, 355-365.

Xunzi (3rd c. BCE/1999).  Trans. J. Knoblock.  Changsha, China: Hunan People’s Publishing House.

Yaffe, G. (2002).  Reconsidering Reid’s geometry of visible.  Philosophical Quarterly, 52, 602–20.

Yamanaka, T., Morita, Y., and Matsumoto, J. (1982).  Analysis of the dream contents in college students by REM-awakening technique.  Folia Psychiatrica et Neurologica Japonica, 36, 33-52.

Yazmajian, R.V. (1964).  Color in dreams.  Psychoanalytic quarterly, 33, 176-193.

Yazmajian, R.V. (1968).  Dreams completely in color.  Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 16, 32-47.

Yost, Jr., R.M., and Kalish, D. (1955).  Miss MacDonald on sleeping and waking.  Philosophical Quarterly, 5, 109-124.

Young, F.A. (1948).  The projection of after-images and Emmert’s Law.  Journal of General Psychology, 39, 161-166.

Zhuangzi (Chuang Tzu) (3rd c. BCE/1968).  The complete works of Chuang Tzu, trans. B. Watson.  New York: Columbia.

Zuckerman, M. (1969).  Hallucinations, reported sensations, and images.  In Sensory deprivation, ed. J.P. Zubek.  New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.